Development of Clinical Discourse Analysis
1. Developed while working in the Albuquerque
Public Schools (1976-1978)
2. Recognized that the contemporary tests and
sampling procedures were poor
3. Determined that more attention to function
rather than superficial aspects of
language was needed.
4. Initiated a data-grounded and interpretive
Research Project
5. Created a set of criteria to identify individuals
who appeared to exhibit language
difficulties.
Had to meet three criteria:
Diagnosed as language disordered by professionals using state standards
Recognized as having poor social skills by their teachers
Poor (below grade level) academic performance
6. Identified a large number of participants
(N>30?) Six years to 22 years of age
7. Collected Large data samples from each in
authentic conversational dyads
8. Transcribed the samples and then cycled
through them listening and identifying
behaviors
that were problematic (seemed to interfere with the conversation) these
would serve as potential functional indices of difficulty
9. Isolated each of these behaviors:
labeled them and wrote a definition for each.
10. Had 27 problematic behaviors at this time
11. Created and applied a set of criteria to "refine" these
behaviors
-- Had to have
occurred in at least two participants
-- Had to have
psychological reality (others could agree and easily ID them)
-- Had to meet
at least one of the following:
* the behavior is a skill that should develop in normal communicators by
4 years
of age.
* the behavior had been independently identified as a characteristic of
conversational difficulty by researchers
* the behavior was found to have some discriminative power as demonstrated
by
some experiments by Damico
12. Reduced or refined the behaviors down to
17
13. Searched for a theoretical Framework to
employ and to anchor the descriptive
behaviors to an acceptable theory (H.P. Grice's Cooperative
Principle)
14. Asked two groups (SLPs and Linguists) to
conduct a "forced choice" and place the
behaviors under one of Grice's four Conversational Maxims.
15. Designed a procedure for "other behaviors"
that might be identified
16. Field tested the procedure and developed
interpretive screens for rich interpretation
17. Collected some data on occurrence of "temporal
mapping problems" in typical
communicators to determine "normal range of variation".
18. First presented at ASHA as a miniseminar
in 1980. Published in 1985.
The Cooperative Principle
(H.P. Grice, 1975)
Categories of the Cooperative Principle
1. Quantity:
The quantity of information to be provided.
A. Make your contribution as informative as is required
B. Don't make the contribution more informative than is required
C. Conciseness
2. Quality:
Try to make your contribution one that is true.
A. Do not say what you believe to be false and represent it as true
B. Do not say for which you lack adequate evidence
3. Relation: Be relevant.
A. Your contribution to conversation should be appropriate to the immediate
needs
at each stage of the transaction.
4. Manner: This is the
act of relating not to what has been said but, rather, to HOW
what is said is to be said.
A. Avoid Obscurity of expression
B. Avoid Ambiguity
C. Be brief and orderly
CLINICAL DISCOURSE ANALYSIS: Definitions
QUANTITY CATEGORY
1. Informational Redundancy
( RED).
This involves the continued and inappropriate fixation on a proposition.
The
speaker will continue to stress a point or relate a fact even when
the listener has
acknowledged its reception and tried to proceed.
Example Examiner: ...any way, I'm glad you enjoy
the fair. Let's talk about
something else. How do/
"Did you ever see the bicentennial fair?
Examiner: No, didn't see it. Hey, do you like your teacher?
"She's really OK. She lets me work on my bulletin
board...she also lets me play with the cars."
Examiner: The cars? Which cars are those?
"The model cars in the state fair exhibit. How much do you really
like the state fair?"
2. Failure to Provide
Significant Information to the Listener (FSI).
The speaker does not
provide the amount or type of information needed by the
listener
for comprehension.
Example Examiner: So, how would I get to your house
from here?
"Turn right there where we play baseball and my house is down a
little bit."
3. The Use of Nonspecific
Vocabulary (NSV).
The speaker uses deictic
terms such as "this", "that", "then", "there", pronominals,
proper nouns, and possessives
when no antecedent or referent is available in the
verbal or nonverbal
context. Consequently, the listener has no way of knowing what
is being referenced.
Individuals displaying this difficulty also tend to overuse generic
terms such as "thing"
and stuff" when more specific information is required.
Example Examiner: Well then, what is your favorite toy?
"My favorite thing is...oh, stuff"
4. Need for Repetition
(NR).
Repetition is required
prior to any indication of comprehension in spite of the fact
that the material
is not apparently difficult.
Example Examiner: What did the little boy
do then?
"........................."
Examiner: What did the boy do then?
"...wh...What?"
Examiner: When he saw this (points to picture) what did the boy do?
"he ran"
QUALITY CATEGORY
5. Message Inaccuracy
(MI).
An attempted communication
involves the relating of inaccurate info.
RELATION CATEGORY
6. Poor Topic
Maintenance (PTM).
The speaker makes
rapid and inappropriate changes in the topic without providing
transitional cues
to the listener.
Example "...but I missed it (an early T.V. program)
cuz I went to bed."
Examiner: That early? You must have had a hard day.
"Yeah"
Examiner: What made it such a hard day?
"the raking"
Examiner: That's hard work isn't it?
"Our teacher said, uh...whoever wins in checkers--I won!--goes
to McDonald."
7. Inappropriate Response
(IR).
The individual makes
a response that indicates a radically unpredictable
interpretation of meaning. It is as though the individual were operating
on an
independent discourse agenda.
Example Examiner: How do you like school?
"I don't know him yet."
8. Failure to
Ask Relevant Questions (FQ).
The individual does
not seek clarification of information that is unclear.
Consequently, there is little or no "verbal play" or clarification
if the message
received from the speaker is unclear or too difficult for the individual
to
comprehend.
9. Situational
Inappropriateness.(SI).
The behavior tends
to account for a generalized lack of relevance. The speaker's
utterance is not only irrelevant to the discourse, but it may also occur
in an
inappropriate social or interactional situation.
Example Examiner: Come in and sit down, Rich. It's nice
to meet you.
"Why does twenty years go by so fast?"
Examiner: Pardon, what did you say?
"Why does twenty years go by so fast?
Examiner: .....Why do you ask that question?
"I don't know...Do you always blink your eyes like that?"
10. Inappropriate
Speech Style (ISS).
The speaker does not
change the structural, lexical, or prosodic form of his
utterances according to the needs of the audience or the context.
This may
involve the occurrence of dialectal structural forms, code switching, style-
shifting, language transfer, or interlanguage phenomena or idiosyncratic
language codes
MANNER CATEGORY
11. Linguistic
Nonfluency (LNF).
The speaker's production
is disrupted by repetitions, unusual pauses, and hesitation
phenomena.
Example "sh...uh..she..um..she comes at dinner."
12. Revision
Behaviors.(R).
The speaker seems
to come to dead ends in a maze, as if starting off in a certain
direction, then coming back to a starting point and beginning anew after
each
attempt. There may be false starts and self-interruptions.
Example "Well, you see...if you want-- sometimes
when you ca--
a lot of times when you can't go out, you can just play
with your twin brothers."
13. Delays Before
Responding (DR).
Communicative exchanges
initiated by others are followed by pauses of inordinate
length at turn-switching points. This does include delays in responding
to
questions.
Example Examiner: Well, what did you do at recess?
"........played tag."
14. Failure
to Structure Discourse (DS).
This problematic
behavior is the most global of the 17. It occurs when the discourse
of the speaker lacks forethought and organizational planning. Due
to this
characteristic, the discourse is confusing--even if all of the propositional
content
is present.
15. Turn-Taking
Difficulty (TTD).
The participant in
a conversational interaction does not attend to the cues necessary
for the appropriate exchange of conversational turns. This results
in one of two
possible outcomes. First, the individual does not allow others to add information.
This is characterized by interruptions or consistent/inappropriate bids
for a turn
Example Examiner: Well I think that the be/
"I like the green one best."
Examiner: Yes, that's a nice one. How about the red one?" Do
you wa/
"Can we find more like this one?"
The second possibility involves an opposite reaction. Rather than always
bidding
for a turn, this individual does not read the switching cues appropriately
and
therefore, does not hold up his/her part of the interaction.
Example Examiner: ...so this school year is almost over.
"...it's not over though is it?"
Examiner: Not over, no it's not.
"What do you mean by that?"
Examiner: Well, most of the year is gone...but not all of it.
You know?
"..................."
16. Gaze Inefficiency
(GI).
The individual's use of eye contact is inconsistent or uncharacteristic
of gaze
utilized in the mainstream cultural context used as the criterion
during this
assessment. Consequently, the attempts to "color" communication or
help
direct the interaction with gaze are inappropriate or ineffective.
17. Inappropriate
Intonational Contour (IC).
The speaker's ability to embellish or "color" or contextualize his meaning
through linguistic/tonal suprasegmentals such as pitch levels, vocal intensity
and other inflectional contours is ineffective or inappropriate.
GUIDELINES FOR USING CLINICAL DISCOURSE ANALYSIS
______________________________________________________________________________
ELICITATION AND DATA COLLECTION
1. It is essential that the data collection involve conversational
interaction. This
procedure is designed to focus on the give-and-take
of conversation and not picture
description, narratives, question-and-answer formats
or close-ended interviews.
2. It is important that the conversational partner (of the targeted
individual) play by the
actual "rules of conversation" and function
as a true conversationalist. From a practical
standpoint this might mean:
-- both partners might provide
a large amount of talk
-- most topics will naturally
arise and develop
-- you don't violate "felicity
conditions". For example, you don't feign ignorance of
something that you know or you don't ask the targeted individual to perform
redundant or meaningless tasks.
-- Much of what is discussed comes
from the "here and now" or from everyday or
recent past experiences.
-- Share as much information yourself
as you expect the targeted individual to share.
3. When addressing the issue of "how much data to collect?", don't
focus on the idea of
obtaining a sufficient number of utterances.
Rather, focus on AUTHENTICITY of the
data. That is, how much confidence do you
have that the data sampled really is an
indication of the individual's typical performance
and his/her best performance? There
are some guidelines that can typically be
used to increase the potential of authentic
data:
-- Collect data from a full range
of speech events
-- Collect data from recurrent
instances of those events
-- Collect data using several
data collection approaches.
Never rely on Clinical Discourse Analysis
by itself to provide you with the data
needed to describe the meaning-making proficiency
of an individual.
4. Authenticity is not determined by a number of utterances or
contexts, it is determined by
your degree of judgment and confidence is
the representativeness of the behavior. It is
best to collect conversational data from several
instances of conversation. The fewer
samples and contexts sampled, the greater
the chance of non-authenticity.
5. Don't think of context as only involving physical setting. Contextual
variables also
include
-- social setting
-- overt behaviors used to frame
and organize
-- language phenomena spoken by
both parties
-- extra situational variables
including background knowledge, experiences and
motivations.
6. The context is very dynamic and frequently changes instanteously.
7. The conversational interaction should be tape-recorded so that it
can be transcribed and
analyzed at a later time.
SEGMENTATION OF THE SAMPLE
1. The segmentation is accomplished using two types of data from
the sample:
A. Phonological Unit - a segment of speech separated by
the contours of intonation,
stress, and
a pause in the subject's voice. This is usually marked by a definite pause
preceded by
a diminishing of force and a drop in pitch of voice (or a rise for
queries).
B. Communication Unit - a segment of speech containing
and defined by the presence
of a proposition that provides
conceptual information (as expressed by a predicate
-- explicit or implied --
taking one or more argument). Typically, this unit can't be
broken down or further divided without loss of essential meaning
(but see note
2.2 below).
2. The phonological and communication units usually co-occur. When
this is not the case,
always segment according
to the meaning component (communication unit).
#Meaning#
(terminal juncture, meaning unit, tj) = 1 unit
#Meaning, Meaning#
= 2 units of transcription
#partial meaning#
Meaning# = 1 unit of transcription
#partial meaning,
partial meaning, Meaning# = 1 unit
2.1- Communication Units need not be complete structurally
"Where are you
going?" "Home"
(1 unit)
2.2- Compound sentences that contain only 1 conjoining
device and that fall within a
phonological
unit are counted as one utterance (e.g., "and", "then", "and then").
"I'm going to the store and I will buy bread"
2.3- Conjoined sentences that have undergone subject or
predicate deletion are counted
as one utterance.
"I was coming home but ran into a big snowstorm"
2.4- Adjoined utterances are counted as one utterance provided
they are within one
phonological
unit.
--Temporal
link: before, after, when, while
"After
its put in cans, its shipped out"
--Casual
link: because
"She
let me stay because I was broke"
--Conditional
link: if
"If
its cold we might have a frost"
--Purposive
link: so
"The
oil repels water so it can swim"
--Disjunctive
link: instead, although, but
"Instead
of getting some sleep, they went fishing"
--Parenthicals
and asides are counted separately
"I
have a pretty star (it blinks!) on my tree"
TRANSCRIPTION OF THE SAMPLE
1. Preserve the speaker-hearer dyad. This means collecting and transcribing
behavior
from both interactants.
It is helpful to do so in a regular format.
2. Retain all non-linguistic contextual information.
3. Preserve the contextualization cues
4. Utilize the key on the form as a standard coding procedure.
5. Number the line in the transcription.
6. Place different transcription units on different lines.
7. Keep the transcription accurate across all interactants.
8. Do not omit any "verbal segments" that seem incomplete. These
false starts and
mazes are important
indices of temporal mapping problems.
CODING OF THE BEHAVIORS IN THE SAMPLE
1. It is essential that all behaviors are coded. Do not engage in the
process of
interpretation at this stage.
2. Use the standard codes for the problematic behaviors (See form).
3. Use the Clinical Discourse Analysis definitions to identify
and code the problematic
behaviors.
4. Place the analysis on the actual transcript. The standard symbology
(see form) should
be used and
the behavior should be coded by placing the symbol over the behavior. 5.
Remember that description and identification of problematic difficulties
is the primary
object of the analysis.
If a behavior can be validly described by the use of two
symbols, then
use them. It results in more subtle description.
6. If there is a problematic behavior that cannot be coded with one
of the CDA
behaviors, code and
document it using the following strategy:
A. Identify the behavior and describe it in writing.
B. Provide a descriptive term that uniquely identifies
it.
C. Determine which Gricean category it best fits.
D. Add the problematic behavior and code it with the others
7. After initial coding, it is frequently helpful to listen to the
tape again while treading the
transcript. Check
your transcription and your coding -- particularly the temporal
mapping problems.
8. Beware of the tendency for "prescriptionism" in coding. You
must code what is present,
but don't be overly judgmental.
9. There are a number of "pitfalls" in the behavioral coding. Some
information to assist in
accurate coding is listed
below by problematic behaviors:
Informational Redundancy
-- Key: "Continued and inappropriate fixation".
-- Usually revealed as a limited number of topics or a limited
repertoire of topics.
-- Grammatical redundancy is not informational redundancy.
-- Don't confuse a repeat for clarification or emphasis or re-establishment
of a previous
topic as this behavior.
-- A repeat is not a fixation.
Failure to Provide Significant Information
-- Remember to focus on content not form. For example, a complete
sentence is not
necessary for meaning transmission.
Ellipsis is not a problematic behavior. Don't
code it as one.
-- This behavior is most easily observed when asking for specific
information or when
the individual spontaneously
provides directions or instructions.
-- Note antecedents or linguistic context to provide information.
This is acceptable.
-- Key: If you know what the child is talking about...even
if the information doesn't
appear to be
available, then it is not a problematic behavior.
Use of Nonspecific Vocabulary
-- Generic terms are acceptable for glossing over.
-- Generic terms are acceptable when utilizing nonverbal or other
contextual information.
-- The key phrase in the definition is "when no antecedent or
referent is available"
-- Make certain that you only code initial instances of nonspecific
pronominals.
-- Key: If you know what the child is talking about...even
if the information doesn't
seem to be available, then
it is not a problematic behavior.
Need for Repetition
-- When you have asked a question and you must
rephrase, restate, or move to
another question or topic,
this is typically a manifestation of this behavior.
Message Inaccuracy
-- Key: Inaccuracy or mis-information
-- The question has been addressed but the response (though a
potential response) is
inaccurate
-- May note this as an obvious contradiction of the context
-- It doesn't matter if the person believes that he is relaying
false information or not......
the key is inaccuracy.
-- Incorrect answers in class are coded here.
-- Not a gauge of moral integrity.
-- Confabulation within the same question frame is coded here
Poor Topic Maintenance
-- This can also be due to a poor transition as well as no transition....perhaps
its triggered
by a somewhat related idea.
-- Typically find that the topics cannot be sustained -- even
when attempted
-- Distractibility may be one "general feeling" when this is
a problem
-- If there is a feeling of "losing your verbal feet", note whether
this is due to a rapid or
inappropriate
topic shift
-- Even if a reason for the shift is obvious, if it was a poor
transition, code it now and
interpret later.
Inappropriate Response
-- Key: Focus is on a plausible response
-- Inaccurate responses that are within the same question frame
are not coded here. See
message inaccuracy
-- The key phrase is "radically unpredictable interpretation"
-- if you must reassert the propositional Question or it is lost
-- code it here
-- When this relational problem comes after a query, code it
here. This is a way to
distinguish between PTM, SI and IR.
Failure to ask relevant Questions
-- This is based more on lack of behavior
-- More interpretive
-- Ask the question: Does the individual try to obtain
clarification or make repair when
he doesn't catch the question
or doesn't understand the verbal environment?
-- Types of behaviors linked to this lack of clarification would
be:
no response
inappropriate
response after a delay
repetition
of Q or of his previous utterance
stereotypic
responses
non-attentiveness
Situational Inappropriateness
-- A generalized lack of relevance
-- A more all-encompassing category
-- due to social or interactional inappropriateness
-- may be reflected in distinct social "faux pas" or extreme
boorishness
-- often does not read social contextualization cues
-- violations of social constraints
-- A more "catch-all" category
Inappropriate Speech Style
-- This is the category for coding difficulties in the superficial
aspects of
language structure
-- Code switching that is inappropriate is coded here.
-- Language transfer phenomena and dialectal differences
are coded under this category.
-- Idiosyncratic terms and language structure coded here.
-- Inappropriate speech registers and style shifting are included
here.
Linguistic Nonfluency
-- Major caution: Count all behaviors. Use a "trips the ear"
criteria.
-- Beware of multiple coding
-- temporally contiguous instances are counted as one instance.
Particularly if they
haven't moved past some
propositional information.
-- Note that there are filled LNFs.
-- Review the tape after transcription to help find these
-- We all produce these behaviors. It appears that the frequency
of occurrence is the
key to difficulty. You must
code all instances and then interpret.
Revisions
-- "Mazing" is a key descriptor
-- Even phoneme revisions are noted and coded.
Delays before Responding
-- Can have a filled DR
-- Simply code the occurrences. Issues of thoughtful
pauses, multiple questions,
complex syntax and processing
thought-provoking queries will be handled in the
interpretation phase.
-- Nonfluency before answering a question is a DR
Structuring Discourse
-- Usually suprasentential
Turn Taking Difficulty
-- there can be two types:
Violating constraints and cues
to inappropriately take the turn
Not upholding the turn
-- Violations of Adjacency principles coded here
-- Code all turn breaks.
-- Note if an individual continues on inappropriately when overlap
occurs.
-- Don't place back-channels or assessment reactions here.
Gaze Inefficiency
-- This is very culturally determined.
-- Focus on what the context requires and then interpret after
you code according to
the context.
Inappropriate Intonational Contour
-- This does include intonational performance due to motoric
problems.
-- Flat or inconsistent contour typical occurrences
-- Key is when the result is mixed messages or inefficient contextualizing
INTERPRETATION OF THE DATA
Key Concepts:
Describing Authentic Social Action
Pragmatic Mapping
Forced Adaptations
Patterns and Systematicity
Subtle Complexity
1. This phase is HEAVY on your knowledge and informed judgment.
Don't be
intimidated. Exposure and experience
will make you better.
** Read the
literature
** Analyze some
normals
** Gain experience
with procedure
** Embrace the
complexity
2. Once you gain more experience, the screens an be employed quickly
and effectively.
3. There must be a framework built into the assessment process that
allows the SLP to
account for conversational complexity.
4. It is not enough to recognize the fact that problematic performance
may be due to
differences or strategic reasons as well as deficits.......
you must be able to
systematically account for this possibility
in a way that is "institutionalized".
5. There is a tendency for prescriptionism in interpretation as well....use
the
questions/screens listed below to help you
prevent this tendency from influencing
your interpretations.
6. Don't be too concerned if you miss behaviors. The pattens
will reveal themselves if
you are systematic and if problems actually
do exist.
7. You may use both qualitative (discussion of patterns and behaviors)
and quantitative
(numbers and percentages) data. Don't
over-extend their interpretive power.
** numbers alone can't determine
level of difficulty or whether or not there is a
problem or disability.
** the patterns and types of behaviors
are more important
** the changes in occurrences of behaviors
may be important
8. It is key to remember that the behaviors on Clinical Discourse Analysis
do serve as
effective indices for potential problems during
conversation. They may serve as "road
maps" telling you where to look for problems.
They may be viewed as potential
"Forced Adaptations". But this is an interpretation
accomplished with completion of
the bi-level analysis paradigm.
Two Distinct
Levels of Analysis
A. Descriptive
Analysis Phase
Focus on Overt Behaviors that may serve as Indices of Problems
Functional Difficulties and Strengths and Adjustments
Clinical Discourse Analysis is one such procedure
Identify and Code all Problematic Behaviors
Look for Patterns
1) Collect all the data
2) Move through data and note co-occurrences
3) Determine numbers and percentages
4) Determine whether there is enough to consider potential problems
* Do these behaviors interfere with social activities
* Do they bother others
* Are there specific and troubling patterns
* Are there sufficient occurrences
* Are the types of behaviors unusual
* Do they prevent success of the three criteria
Effectiveness
Appropriateness
Fluency
5) If problems exist from the perspective of the context of interest,
then move to the second analysis level.
B. Explanatory
Analysis Phase
Focus on Causes and ask Why do the problematic behaviors occur?
This process typically involves the combining of LOW INFERENCE data
(observational data, test results, interview information on background,
etc)
to form HIGH INFERENCE JUDGMENTS.
There are sets of questions and considerations that can assist in addressing
this
process . This process will result in the following:
* A summary of the data collected
* A deeper interpretation of the data to comment on
1) whether there are difficulties
2) what areas of functioning are affected
3) whether these difficulties are due to impairment
4) what contextual variables are significant
5) what is the student's intervention potential
Process
1. Take each of the problematic behaviors (individually or the pattern)
2. Analyze it (them) to see why they occur
3. Start with an assumption of normalcy
4. Apply the questions of screens detailed below
9. The following Explanatory Questions/Screens are designed to force
you to consider
the potential causal factors for the occurrence
of the problematic behaviors in the
conversational sample. These are designed
into several general categories of
consideration. Proceed in the following
order. This will prevent you from mistakes
due to a prescriptionistic bias.
Screen One: Are the problematic behaviors
manifestations of Normal
Conversational Strategies?
-- Interactional strategies
are often used to "opt" out or to maintain control.
Ambiguity (FSI....RED...NR)
-- Defense of Face
-- Discourse markers to
gain control (LNF....R)
-- Avoidance manifested
as Poor Topic Maintenance
-- Word Search as attention
getting device
-- Forgetting as an interactional
resource
-- Nonfluency to maintain
the floor
-- Mid-gaze or "Thinking
Face" to signal need for co-participation
-- Message inaccuracy as
bantering or to get a reaction
Screen Two: Are the problematic behaviors merely in random
variation or
operating within normal limits?
-- Temporal Mapping Problems
are especially relevant here (Linguistic Nonfluency,
Revisions, Delays before Responding)
** Normal range of occurrence (6% - 24% of utterances)
** Can increase these in all of us by operating outside of the level
of
comprehensibility and/or comfort
** Best to note significant change in occurrence with Concomitant change
in
level of complexity
-- There does not seem to
be a pattern or repeated occurrences
** Simple communicative breakdown
-- Delays before responding
as a reaction to multiple questions, thought-provoking
queries, complex syntax and processing.
Screen Three: Are the problematic behaviors due to procedural mistakes
by the
assessor?
-- Incorrect Coding
Procedures
** Too Prescriptionistic or biased
** Multiple NSV on pro-nominals
** FSI for lack of lexical specificity despite comprehension
** Message inaccuracies as Inappropriate Responses
-- Violation of Sincerity
Constraints
-- Operated beyond the level
of comprehensibility
** Give Rise to Temporal Mapping Problems
-- Not a conversational
interaction
-- Inappropriate contextualization
cues
Screen Four: Are the problematic behaviors due to language
or dialectal differences
between the individual and the language code of the context of
interest?
-- Inappropriate speech
style due to VBE or code switching
-- Culture of Poverty and
"restricted code"
-- Note the problems in
L2 but not in L1
-- Due to interlanguage
phenomena
-- Due to normal L2 acquisition
phenomena
-- Due to language transfer
errors
Screen Five: Are the problematic behaviors due to cultural
differences/
interferences that affect interactional expectancies or strategies?
-- Different eye gaze
-- Different nonverbal contextualization
-- Different clarification
strategies
-- Silence as a conversational
device
Screen Six: Is there an indication of extreme test anxiety
during the observational
assessment in one context but not in others?
Screen Seven: Is there significant performance inconsistency
between different
observation periods in the same context?
Screen Eight: Have any contextual variables of significant been identified?
Screen Nine: Is there any evidence that the problematic
behaviors noted can be
explained according to any bias effect that was in operation before,
during, or after the assessment?
-- Is the student in a subtractive
bilingual/cultural environment?
-- Is the student a member
of a disempowered community?
-- Are negative or lowered
expectations for this student held by the student, the
student's family, or the educational staff?
-- Were specific indications
of bias evident in the prereferral, referral,
administrative, scoring, or interpretative phases of the evaluation?
Screen Ten: Are the Problematic Behaviors Potential Compensatory
Strategies?
-- "Isy" as a discourse
marker
-- Simmons-Mackie &
Damico, 1996; 1997
Screen Eleven: Are there any remaining (unaccounted for) Problematic
Behaviors
or Patterns of behaviors that reveal an underlying linguistic systematicity
during the descriptive analysis phase?
-- This question applies
only to the problematic behaviors that are still remaining
after application of the ten screens.
-- Isolate turns/utterances
containing remaining problematic behaviors
-- Is there significant
performance inconsistency between different input or output
modalities?
-- Perform a systematic
linguistic analysis on these data points looking for
consistency in appearance of problematic behaviors.
* Grammatical (primarily syntagmatic)
Crystal profiles (1982)
SALT (Miller & Chapman, 1983)
* Semantic (primarily paradigmatic)
PRISM (Crystal, 1982)
Perceptual/Language Distancing
Displacement
Levels of Abstraction
Cohesion/Coherence (Halliday & Hasan, 1976)
-- Look for large changes
in the occurrence of the problematic behaviors as the
complexity of one of these dimensions increases (e.g., an increase in
grammatical complexity from LARSP Level IV to LARSP Level V causes an
increase in the number of linguistic nonfluencies from 18% to 39%.
10. Determine Appropriate Placement
* A plan of action detailing what further
service delivery should occur. This should
include:
1. What should be
done
2. Who should do it
3. When services are
provided
4. What support will
be provided
5. How follow-up is
accomplished
Click
here to return to Dr. Damico's Homepage
Click
here to return to Dr. Damico's Professional Page